Periodic Reporting for period 2 - AWAR (Adapted to War)
Período documentado: 2022-10-01 hasta 2024-03-31
To probe the existence of the adaptation, we have designed a research program consisting of the following steps (or objectives):
1. Reconstruct the setting of pre-Holocene war.
2. Identify an adaptive problem in the setting of pre-Holocene war.
3. Theorize an adaptation that may help solve this problem.
4. Specify the features of the adaptation via a task analysis.
5. Based on these design features, derive testable hypotheses.
6. Test for the existence of such features (which tests for the existence of the adaptation itself).
7. Examine cross-cultural variation in the outputs of the adaptation.
8. Explore contemporary implications of the adaptation: does it shape people's attitudes toward, and behavior in, modern war or political violence more generally?
1. If war occurred before the Holocene, it took the form of hit-and-run ambushes or surprise hunting encounters.
2. If such ambushes or encounters occurred, then ancestral hominins regularly faced the problem of how not to fall victim to a coalitional attack upon a surprise inter-group encounter.
3. Solving this problem required a psychological adaptation that facilitates detection and evaluation of outgroups upon surprise encounters.
4. The specific features of this adaptation were theorized using a task analysis, an analytical tool borrowed from vision research. An evolutionary task analysis starts by defining the end state and proceeds in specifying the concrete tasks involved in achieving that end state. In this case, the end state is surviving a surprise intergroup encounter. We have identified multiple such tasks, e.g.:
- Task 1: "Detect the outgroup coalition"
- Task 2: "Determine the size of the outgroup coalition"
Each task is accompanied by testable hypotheses, e.g.:
- Task 1: People attend, automatically (non-intentionally), to potential outgroup coalitions (H1)
- Task 2: People determine, accurately (H2) and rapidly (H3), the size of potential outgroup coalitions (i.e. the number of individuals in the coalition)
To test the hypotheses (i.e. Step 6), we have developed and fine-tuned experimental designs based on extant dot-probe and dot-enumeration paradigms. These experimental designs have been adapted for online administration and have already been piloted, providing evidence to support the project's core hypotheses. The theoretical ideas and the research designs with results have been evaluated by leading scholars across multiple research labs (e.g. the Center of Evolutionary Psychology at the University of California, Santa Barbara) and international conferences (e.g. the Society for Neuroscience annual meeting).
1. Developed an elaborate information-processing model of an adaptation to coalitional aggression.
2. This model has produced 13 novel hypotheses, predicting the existence of certain tendencies in the human mind that allow effectively navigating coalitional aggression.
3. Some hypotheses are general, pertaining to processes studied by scholars in multiple research fields. For example, the theorizing about coalitional formidability assessments has led to the prediction that our visual attention system spontaneously attends to coordinated groups of males and that our enumeration capacity is enhanced when the enumerated objects resemble coordinated groups of males.
Methods:
4. We have developed and extensively piloted several experimental designs. We built on existing paradigms in cognitive psychology but also considerably improved upon them in original ways. Specifically, we have adapted the dot-probe and dot-enumeration paradigms to assess the specific features people attend to in human groups (e.g. sex or posture coordination) and how people enumerate naturalistic objects (groups of humans), as contrasted to abstract objects (e.g. dots). We have tested multiple design choices (e.g. stimuli presentation times) which improve the reliability of both paradigms. Furthermore, the generic dot-probe and dot-enumeration paradigms had typically been used in experiments administered in physical laboratories. We have developed procedures, with publicly available code, to administer these tasks online, e.g. via crowdsourcing platforms such as Prolific. The shift from lab to online research has been conditioned by the COVID-19 pandemic. The pandemic has been a major logistical obstacle and, at the same time, a major cause of innovation for AWAR. Pandemic-related restrictions rendered lab research in many cases unfeasible. Because of this, technologies and methodologies have developed rapidly to administer experiments online. The project has capitalized on these developments and by now has produced a number of innovations in the form of cognitive experimental tasks that can be feasibly administered online. To reach these goals, a highly-skilled research team was recruited, including social psychologists, cognitive neuroscientists, and behavioral economists, who have brought a unique mix of skills to the project, from advanced programming of experiments to in-depth expertise in human visual attention. We have also collaborated with scholars from the research-site countries, including Ukraine.
Findings:
5. To pilot the experimental designs and to test the hypotheses, we have completed 25 exploratory online studies (hereafter "Pilots"), in which a total of 5222 individuals already took part. In line with our hypotheses, we found preliminary evidence that people automatically attend to coordinated male groups, accurately and rapidly determine their size (i.e. the N of individuals in the group), and use the size of groups to inform subjective formidability assessments. We are currently finalizing the submission of the main preregistered study, which will test the hypotheses using large nationally representative samples. We have also drafted designs to test the project's hypotheses in ecologically-valid ways with participants from countries experiencing armed conflict, as well as designs for large multinational study, spanning 40 countries.